In this discussion, I have suggested several reasons why drawing or painting is difficult and why the productions of children and of artists of antiquity contain certain kinds of "errors." However, I must add an important disclaimer. Drawings or paintings that are poor representations of objects and scenes by our standards and expectations of realism need not be the result of the perceptual or cognitive limitations I have suggested; rather, they may be the result of an intention or convention to draw in a particular way. Perhaps the Egyptians drew pictures of people the way they did because they believed that side views of faces and feet and frontal views of torsos were the most informative about the human body. The lack of perspective in painting does not necessarily mean the absence of the ability to use it. The realistic aim is not the only aim in art, and it only appears here and there in different cultures and at different times in history. Similarly, children are not always concerned with making realistic drawings but instead may be motivated to express themselves in form and color. It would be absurd to infer from Picasso’s more abstract paintings that he could not draw realistically when some of his other work shows how great a master of realistic portrayal he was when he chose to be. If one did not know about the realistic paintings, however, one might make just that wrong inference from the abstract ones. In this and the preceding chapters, I have necessarily referred often to object and shape (or form) perception. From this one might draw the conclusion that such perception is based solely on the reception of the shape of the object’s image on the retina as given by its contours. As will be seen in the next chapter, nothing could be farther from the truth. Even so simple a perception as that of a single two-dimensional shape poses many difficult problems.